A general and abstract account of something
Neu-Ulm University of Applied Sciences
August 29, 2024
Form small groups, have a look at your notes on Podsakoff, MacKenzie, and Podsakoff (2016) and synthesize your answers to the following questions:
A concept is an abstract idea, notion, or mental representation that represents a specific category of objects, events, behaviors, or phenomena (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, and Podsakoff 2016).
Concepts are fundamental to human cognition and communication and to research in particular.
Science is built on the cumulative advancement of knowledge that requires rigor.
In this regard, concepts enable e.g.
collaboration,
theoretical development,
empirical research &
theory testing
Podsakoff, MacKenzie, and Podsakoff (2016) propose a set of recommendations for creating better concept definitions:
clear and precise,
differentiated,
explicit &
theoretically founded
A theory is a general and abstract account of something.
Many parents believe that the right name leads to economic prosperity
A study of California birth registry data from all year since 1961 shows that the name indeed correlates with economic prosperity.
How can that be explained?
Names are not the cause, they are just one manifestation of an underlying reason.
Sutton and Staw (1995) define theory as a comprehensive framework that goes beyond simple descriptions, hypotheses, data, or metaphors. A true theory provides a systematic and explanatory understanding of a set of phenomena, offering insights into the underlying mechanisms and causal relationships.
Sutton and Staw (1995) argue that theory is often misunderstood and misused in academic and scholarly contexts. They provide a clear distinction between what theory is and what it is not:
Universal, data, descriptive, hypotheses, models, design
Theories guide and give meaning to what we see.
Theory is something we use all the time in our everyday life to construct explanations about the world in which we live.
What is specific to scientific theories?
According to Bacharach (1989), a scientific theory
Analysis
Explanation
Prediction
Explanation & prediction
Design & action
The “what” of theories.
The “how” of theories.
Propositions are associations postulated at the theoretical level (they relate concepts), hypotheses are tested at the empirical level (they relate constructs) (Whetten 1989).
The “why” of theories.
The logic provides the basis for justifying the propositions as postulated (Whetten 1989).
Without logic, propositions will be ad hoc, random, and meaningless
The constraints of theories.
Just as a collection of words does not make a sentence, a collection of constructs and variables does not make a theory. Bacharach (1989, 496)
Technology acceptance
Providing an explanation of IT acceptance
The Technology Acceptance Model (TAM) (Davis 1989)
Form small groups, research the technology acceptance model and dissect its building blocks.
Theory of Reasoned Action (TRA)—an established theory in psychology to understand an individual’s behavior (Ajzen 1985).
The Technology Acceptance Model (TAM) (Davis 1989) translates the key tenets of TRA into the IT acceptance domain (except for subjective norms).
Construct | Operational Definition | Variables (Measurement Items) |
---|---|---|
Behavioral intention | Participants intentions to use a particular system in the future | I intend to use the system in the next |
I predict I would use the system in the next |
||
I plan to use the system in the next |
||
Attitude towards Behavior | An individual’s positive or negative feelings about performing the target behavior. | Using the system is a bad/good idea. |
Using the system is a foolish/wise idea. | ||
I dislike/like the idea of using the system. | ||
Using the system is unpleasant/ pleasant. | ||
Perceived Usefulness | The degree to which a person believes that using a particular system would enhance his or her job performance. | Using the system in my job would enable me to accomplish tasks more quickly. |
Using the system would improve my job performance. | ||
Using the system in my job would increase my productivity. | ||
Using the system would enhance my effectiveness on the job. | ||
Using the system would make it easier to do my job. | ||
I would find the system useful in my job. | ||
Perceived Ease of Use | The degree to which a person believes that using a system would be free of effort. | Learning to operate the system would be easy for me. |
I would find it easy to get the system to do what I want it to do. | ||
My interaction with the system would be clear and understandable. | ||
I would find the system to be flexible to interact with. | ||
It would be easy for me to become skillful at using the system. | ||
I would find the system easy to use |
Search for the UTAUT model (Unified Theory of Acceptance and Use of Technology) (Venkatesh et al. 2003) and try to understand how it relates to the TAM.
Theorizing is the application or development of theoretical arguments to make sense of a real account (e.g. an observed phenomenon) (Recker 2021).
Theory is always an simplification
It should not be more complex than the phenomenon to be investigated.
A strong theory is an idealization
From theory to data
Start with a theory, modify/extend it, test the predictions
From data to theory
Start with data, check if there is a theory that can explain what you observe, develop a new theoretical account
Building theory,
testing theory,
and extending theory.
These approaches are not mutually exclusive, and researchers often combine elements from multiple approaches depending on the research context.
The choice depends on the research question, available data, and the goals of the study.
Recker (2021), Mueller and Urbach (2017) and others propose that you have a good theory when you have answer to the following questions:
Start with an observation
Theories should be about classes of things, not the thing (more general)2.
Theories should be explanatory. Moreover, they should be free of circular arguments as circularity prevents theories from being falsifiable.
A good theory is general enough to generate implications for other groups of people and other contexts, all of which serve as potential tests of the theory (i.e., the theory is fertile).
Often, we find that there are also other explanations:
We need to study different contexts to see how our theories explain observations.
Read Hund et al. (2021) and make notes on following questions:
Gregor, Shirley. 2006. “The Nature of Theory in Information Systems.” MIS Quarterly, 611–42.
Sutton, Robert I, and Barry M Staw. 1995. “What Theory Is Not.” Administrative Science Quarterly, 371–84.
Van de Ven, Andrew H. 1989. “Nothing Is Quite so Practical as a Good Theory.” Academy of Management Review 14 (4): 486–89.
Weber, Ron. 2003. “Theoretically Speaking1.” MIS Quarterly 27 (3): III.
Weber, Ron. 2012. “Evaluating and Developing Theories in the Information Systems Discipline.” Journal of the Association for Information Systems 13 (1): 2.
Weick, Karl E. 1989. “Theory Construction as Disciplined Imagination.” Academy of Management Review 14 (4): 516–31.
Weick, Karl E. 1995. “What Theory Is Not, Theorizing Is.” Administrative Science Quarterly 40 (3): 385–90.
The example draws from Lave and March (1993).
Theories that are too narrow and specific (i.e., low-level theories) are not very interesting, even if they are correct.